Growth Curve

The inexact science of raising kids

Laura Sanders

Growth Curve

Growth Curve

Young babies live in a world unto themselves

baby getting tiny shocks on feet

An infant receives a buzz to one foot crossed over the other, an experiment that let scientists study how babies experience the world. 

Sponsor Message

Babies’ minds are mysterious. Thoughts might be totally different in a brain that lacks words, and sensations might feel alien in a body so new. Are babies’ perceptions like ours, or are they completely different?  Even if babies could talk, words would surely fail to convey what it’s like to experience, oh, every single thing for the first time.

A recent paper offers a sliver of insight into young babies’ inner lives. The study, published October 19 in Current Biology, finds an example in which 4-month-old babies are happily oblivious to the external world.

The research focuses on a perceptual trick that suckers adults and 6-month-old babies alike. When the hands are crossed, people often mistake which hand feels a touch. Let’s say your left hand (now crossed over to the right side of your body) gets a tickle. Your eyes would see a hand on the right side of your body get touched — a place usually claimed by your right hand, but now occupied by your left. Those mismatches between sight, touch and expectation can thwart you from quickly and correctly saying which hand was touched.

Here’s the twist: 4-month-old babies don’t fall for this trick, Andrew Bremner of Goldsmiths, University of London and his colleagues found. In the experiment, a researcher would hold infants’ legs in either a crossed position or straight, while one of two remote-controlled buzzers taped to their feet tickled one foot. The researchers then watched which foot or leg wiggled as a result. If the buzzed foot moved, that meant that the baby got it right.

In either the crossed or straight-legged position, 4-month-old babies moved the tickled foot about 70 percent of the time, the team found. But babies just two months older were fooled by the leg cross, moving the tickled foot in about half the trials.

Bremner interprets the results to mean that young babies don’t link the outside visual world to their own bodies yet. Instead, they exist in a state he calls “tactile solipsism,” in which the only thing young babies know about touch is the sensation on the skin, not who or what is causing it. “Your bodily world would be almost completely separate from your visual world,” he says.

This touch/outer world division may help explain other weird things babies do. As babies first learn to move their arms and legs, they’ll hilariously try to stalk and grab one of their own hands with the other. (My husband and I enjoyed narrating this process David Attenborough–style:  “The predatory righthand stalks the elusive left hand prey.”) Young babies seem oblivious to the fact that they own — and can control — their other hand, perhaps because they haven’t yet connected the visual signal coming from the hand to the feeling on their body.

As babies grow and learn more about their world, including the lesson that the things they see are sometimes the things that touch them, their bodies become more integrated with the world. No longer tiny islands, they get better at stitching together sights, sounds, smells and other information into perceptions that make sense. 

Human Development

Why kids look funny when they run

By Laura Sanders 7:00am, October 11, 2015
Kids’ short legs give them little time to push high off the ground, a constraint that leads to the jerky toddler trot.
Human Development,, Language

For kids learning new words, it’s all about context

By Laura Sanders 3:28pm, September 21, 2015
By recording the first three years of life, researchers get hints about a child’s language development.
Human Development

Backwash from nursing babies may trigger infection fighters

By Laura Sanders 11:36am, September 15, 2015
A nursing baby’s saliva may get slurped back into mom’s breast, where it stimulates an immune response.

Five reasons to not totally panic about ticks and Lyme disease

By Meghan Rosen 6:00am, August 21, 2015
We’ve been trained to panic about tick bites and Lyme disease. There are risks to both — and here are some key facts.
Neuroscience,, Human Development

Study finds early signs of bookishness in a child’s brain

By Laura Sanders 3:55pm, August 18, 2015
Children from book-friendly homes show higher brain activity when they hear a story, but there’s more to learn about how reading affects growing brains.
Microbes,, Human Development,, Health

Antibiotics early in life may have lingering effects

By Laura Sanders 11:17am, July 31, 2015
A study in mice show long-lasting effects from courses of antibiotics early in life.
Neuroscience,, Human Development

In children, a sense of time starts early

By Laura Sanders 10:32am, July 16, 2015
Minutes, hours, days and years start to take on new meaning as children acquire a deeper concept of time.
Health,, Science & Society

Should you eat your baby’s placenta?

By Laura Sanders 7:00am, June 26, 2015
More women are choosing to eat their baby’s placenta after giving birth, but the evidence for benefits isn’t there yet.
Language,, Human Development

Unlike moms, dads tend not to coo in squeaky voices

By Meghan Rosen 8:55am, June 12, 2015
American English-speaking moms dial up their pitch drastically when talking to their children, but dads’ voices tend to stay steady, a new study finds.
Health,, Physiology

Why breast-feeding really can be easier the second time around

By Laura Sanders 8:30am, June 3, 2015
The body remembers how to make milk, a mouse study suggests. Something similar may happen in humans.
Subscribe to RSS - Growth Curve