Extreme Climate Survey
Science News is collecting reader questions about how to navigate our planet's changing climate.
What do you want to know about extreme heat and how it can lead to extreme weather events?
Though Central Asia is home to 73 million
people, Western researchers such as Tamis-LeMonda have only recently begun to
document the gahvora’s use and possible impact on how children grow.
Ignoring cultural variation of this sort leaves
a big blind spot in the science of child development. Western researchers and
medical staff define “normal” development — in this case, how and when babies
acquire motor skills such as sitting, crawling and walking — based on a century
of research on mostly white, Western babies.
Now a few motor development experts are pushing
back with a new line of thinking that traces back to the 1950s, when evidence
for huge variations in how and when babies acquire motor skills began to emerge
in a piecemeal way. At that time, anthropologists and cultural psychologists
working in far-flung locales started documenting how babies in different
cultures move about.
In recent decades, that research has become
more systematic. Scientists are comparing the motor skills of babies in various
cultures and creating controlled experiments to see if training can speed up
the development of certain skills.
And motor skills don’t arise in isolation. When
a baby begins to sit, crawl or walk, she gains a new view on the world, which
alters her perception. It also influences how babies and caregivers
communicate. A baby who has learned to walk, for instance, will often carry
objects to her mother, who frequently responds with words or ways of speaking
that are new to the baby. So researchers are also studying how culture
influences other areas of development linked to motor skills.
This research is “not just about walking,” says
Lana Karasik, a developmental psychologist at the City University of New York’s
College of Staten Island. “It’s about what walking gives babies.”
As this work continues among broader
populations, it’s becoming clear that across continents and cultures, children
with the ability to do so will learn to walk. For some babies, that tentative
first step may occur at 8 months old; for others, age 2 or 3 is a perfectly
good time to start exploring.
Babies and toddlers throughout Central Asia spend long stretches restrained inside cradles known as gahvoras (one shown in Tajikistan). Researchers are studying Tajik children to understand the interplay between this cultural practice and motor development. L.B. Karasik et al /PLOS ONE 2018
The rule book
Many parents in Western cultures are familiar
with infant motor development charts. Three-month-olds might be shown lifting
their heads, 6-month-olds are sitting and 12-month-olds are walking. The
implication is that babies learn to get around on a relatively fixed timeline,
regardless of environment or experience.
Such charts trace their origins to the early
1900s, when developmental psychologist Arnold Gesell of Yale University began
filming babies from behind a one-way mirror. Based on 12,000 recordings, Gesell
outlined in 1928 a developmental schedule for babies from 3 to 30 months old.
Meanwhile, psychologist Nancy Bayley began a
study tracking development in more than 60 white babies born to relatively
affluent families in Berkeley, Calif., in the late 1920s. That decades-long
project, known as the Berkeley Growth Study, prompted Bayley to develop a way
for a nonfamily member to assess a child’s development, including motor skills.
She rolled out the Bayley Scales of Infant Development in 1969. Researchers and
clinicians still widely use those scales, now in their fourth iteration.
Gesell, Bayley and others thought that babies
began to move when their bodies matured enough to do so, and that motor skills
emerged along a linear path, with sitting coming before crawling and crawling
before walking. But that thinking hinged on a small set of U.S. babies.
In the early 2000s, the World Health Organization sought to broaden research on motor development to include the rest of the world. WHO researchers measured motor skill acquisition from 4 months to age 2 among 816 babies from five countries: Ghana, India, Norway, Oman and the United States. The analysis, appearing in 2006 in Acta Paediatrica , outlined windows of development during which certain motor skills should arise . Failure to achieve those skills within given windows — 8 to 18 months for walking independently, for instance — was considered “evidence of abnormal growth.”
Unfortunately, the WHO relied on Bayley’s motor
scale, which meant the study used white U.S. babies as the standard of
comparison. Also, the research lacked babies from cultures where scientists
have documented accelerated or uneven patterns of motor development, including
the many cultures of Central Asia.
When “norms” based on a narrow sample of babies
get built into a model and then that model is applied to a different, but still
narrow, sample of babies, the whole system falls apart, says Karen Adolph, a
psychologist at NYU. “Do you really want to say a third of the world is delayed
and another third of the world is accelerated and our part of the world is
normal?”
The need to look beyond the United States was
driven home for Adolph several years ago, when she heard from a woman at
Procter & Gamble who had been tasked with selling diapers throughout
Central Asia. Sales, the woman said, were abysmal. It seemed the gahvora was to
blame.
Adolph relayed the story to her graduate student
Lana Karasik, who was studying motor development across cultures. Karasik
replied that her husband’s family is from the region. “I know that practice,”
she said. So several months later, in early 2014 and in collaboration with
UNICEF and Save the Children, Karasik, Adolph and Tamis-LeMonda launched a
study of motor development in babies in Tajikistan.
Culture clash
As Gesell and Bayley were building their models
of motor development, other researchers had begun to document deviations from
those standards. Charles Super, a developmental psychologist at the University
of Connecticut in Storrs, recalls reading a paper from a researcher studying
Ugandan infants in the 1950s. Ugandan babies walked much earlier than babies in
the West. The researcher wrongly interpreted that difference as an inferiority,
suggesting that fast development would mean intellectual stunting, Super
recalls. “I didn’t like that argument.”
In the 1970s, Super moved to Kenya with his wife, an anthropologist. He began investigating motor development among babies born in a farming community known as Kokwet. Between 1972 and 1975, he documented when those babies acquired new motor skills using the Bayley scale and interviewed mothers about their child-rearing practices.
Kokwet babies sat, stood and walked about a month earlier than Western infants , Super reported in 1976 in Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology . But the babies were slower to master other skills, such as lifting their heads, rolling over and crawling.
Super observed that mothers wore their babies
on their backs while laboring in the fields. He suspected that vigorous motion
gave the babies the sort of constant exercise needed to help develop strength
and agility. The mothers also told Super they actively trained their children
to walk through exercises like air stepping.
“The parents had a theory: If you don’t teach
your children to walk, they won’t walk,” Super says. At the same time, however,
mothers sought to keep their babies from crawling given myriad dangers on the
ground, such as open fire pits and snakes. The training combined with the
restrictions probably explains the development patterns that Super observed
that were outside of normal ranges. His findings agreed with observations made
elsewhere.
For instance, anthropologist Alma Gottlieb’s
research on the Beng people in Ivory Coast from the late 1970s to the early
1990s showed that Beng babies sit earlier than Western babies but are actively
discouraged from walking before age 1. The Beng believe that early walking can
cause a grandparent’s early death, says Gottlieb, a visiting scholar at Brown
University in Providence, R.I. And keeping the babies close and happy
discourages the little ones from returning to a previous life.
Veronique Amenan Akpoueh carries her young grandson on her back as she roasts corn. The Beng people of Ivory Coast believe that infants yearn to return to a previous life. Holding babies constantly is thought to keep them happy so they forget that desire. It’s seen as a way to reduce the risk of infant mortality. A. Gottlieb
When German psychologist Heidi Keller used Bayley’s rubric on the Nso people in Cameroon in the 1990s and 2000s, she found their motor skills were mostly advanced compared with German babies. She attributed the difference to the fact that Nso babies are in constant contact with caregivers and provided with regular exercise and massage. “Every culture emphasizes the domains of development that are considered important,” Keller says.
Motor skills can be acquired “out of order” and
selectively accelerated or decelerated through cultural practices, research by
Super, Gottlieb, Keller and others have shown.
In recent years, researchers have conducted experiments to see if training can accelerate motor skill development. At a public pool in Reykjavík, Iceland, one dynamic swim instructor taught a dozen 3- to 5-month-old babies to stand atop a hand or board — well in advance of the 9-month “norm” for standing, researchers reported in 2017 in Frontiers in Psychology .
One natural, unintended experiment came from
advice from the American Academy of Pediatrics in the 1990s. To reduce the risk
of sudden infant death syndrome, or SIDS, which is more likely to occur in
babies who sleep on their bellies, the academy suggested that babies be placed
on their backs to sleep. But back sleeping delayed when those infants developed
the abilities to roll, sit, crawl and stand. Importantly, studies looking into
the delay found that these babies eventually caught up to their
stomach-sleeping peers. Just in case, the academy now recommends daily tummy
time, where babies play on their stomachs to build strong muscles.
At almost 5 months old, this baby stands on a kickboard at a pool in Reykjavík, Iceland. A dozen 3- to 5-month-olds learned to stand well in advance of their peers — suggesting that training can speed the acquisition of motor skills in babies. H. Sigmundsson, H.W. Lorås and M. Haga/Frontiers in Psychology 2017
Cradled and bound
Rugged, mountainous Tajikistan is bordered by
China, Afghanistan, Uzbekistan and Kyrgyzstan. Following the collapse of the
Soviet Union, the country experienced a civil war. Today, infrastructure
remains poor, with snow and flooding making the winding, mountain roads
impassable for much of the year. Those logistical challenges have limited
Karasik’s research to the capital city of Dushanbe and surrounding villages.
She travels with behavioral scientist Scott Robinson, who got involved with the
work while spending a semester in Adolph’s lab.
Karasik, a Belarusian refugee who moved to the
United States in 1989 at age 10, is well-suited to working in Tajikistan. She
can communicate in Russian, which many Tajiks still speak, fostering a level of
trust in Karasik not often afforded to outsiders. She has also recruited
Dodojonova and other local Tajik women to run her project while she’s
away.
With so little known about gahvora cradles,
especially in rural areas, Karasik’s first order of business was to document
Tajik life. In the villages Karasik and her team visited, families live in
one-room clay huts and share labor and child-care duties with neighbors. Almost
half the fathers work as laborers in Russia and are absent for extended
periods; the rest work odd jobs or are unemployed. Electricity tends to work
for only two hours in the morning and two at night, during which time families
watch television and eat dinner. The gahvora is placed in the center of the
room.
Karasik’s team measured gahvora use through videos and interviews with mothers. All but three of 185 mothers interviewed used a gahvora , the team reported in October 2018 in PLOS ONE . Newborns spent anywhere from 8.5 to 23 hours a day in the cradle; 2-year-olds spent two to 14.5 hours. About 40 percent of mothers breastfed babies while leaning over the gahvora, and 83 percent of the moms engaged in vigorous rocking that lasted anywhere from about four to 22 minutes at a time.
In July 2018, Karasik presented unpublished
research in Philadelphia at the International Congress of Infant Studies
showing that Tajik babies hit motor skill milestones months later than babies
in the WHO study. For instance, at 1 year of age, almost all infants in the WHO
sample were crawling and half were walking. At age 1, just 62 percent of Tajik
babies are crawling and 9 percent are walking. Using WHO standards, almost half
of all Tajik babies would be diagnosed with motor delays, Karasik says.
But Tajik babies seem to catch up to their Western
peers by about age 4 with no discernible long-term repercussions, data
collected by Karasik show. What Karasik really wants to understand moving
forward is how being bound for such long stretches during those early formative
years affects other areas of development and even babies’ temperaments.
VIDEO
A woman in Tajikistan demonstrates how a baby is strapped into a cradle called a gahvora. Babies spend hours immobilized this way, which may affect when they learn to walk.
Walking and talking
The idea that the acquisition of a new motor
skill triggers other skills is known as the developmental cascade. When a baby
acquires a new way of getting around, the child’s vantage point changes, along
with interactions with caregivers and the ability to explore the environment,
says Eric Walle, a developmental psychologist at the University of California, Merced.
Walle is particularly interested in the link between walking and language.
After discovering that babies who can walk have
larger vocabularies than infants who are still crawling, Walle decided to see
what would happen to language skills if he tweaked when babies learned to walk.
But “you can’t really experimentally manipulate walking onset,” he says.
So Walle did the next best thing. He took his
research to Shanghai, where babies typically walk about six weeks later than
U.S. babies. That difference may be because babies in urban China live in more
cramped environments and have less opportunity to move around than U.S. babies.
He compared the language skills of 40 12.5-month-old U.S. babies with 42 Chinese babies, ages 13 to 14.5 months old. Both groups were almost evenly split between walkers and crawlers. His analysis, appearing in 2015 in Infancy , showed that the same divergence in language abilities seen between walking versus crawling American infants also occurs in Chinese babies. In other words, language skills emerge alongside the ability to walk.
“Even though these kids were walking later,
growing up in a very different culture, and exposed to a very different language,
they were showing a similar difference,” Walle says. “Walking shakes up the
system.”
A child’s view
Karasik is keen to see if the gahvora
influences how Tajik babies think. Testing that link in a remote region with a
dodgy supply of electricity has proved challenging, though. For instance, eye
trackers are often used to study how infants view the world around them. But
typical eye trackers are designed to be stationary, which means they’re heavy
and expensive.
Enter visual perception researcher Kirsten Dalrymple
of the University of Minnesota in Minneapolis. Her team has developed a
portable eye tracker that runs on batteries, useful for remote villages.
Dalrymple also had some idea about the areas of development to focus on, such
as the ability to match sights and sounds, which in U.S. babies has been shown
to develop alongside motor ability.
“Our brains have to learn: ‘Hey, every time I
clap my hands together, this noise comes out.’ That’s not something we’re born
with,” Dalrymple says.
Karasik and Dalrymple began by gauging when
American babies develop that ability. Babies come into the lab, and the eye
tracker sits on a nearby table, where it uses a camera to measure reflections
coming off the eye. On a computer screen synced to the tracker, two cartoon
animals jump up and down, but only one is paired with a “doink” sound.
When a baby’s eyes focus only on the animal
making noise, researchers interpret that as the baby correctly pairing sights
and sounds. An unpublished pilot study of 30 babies in Minnesota suggests that
pairing ability appeared at an age of around 9 months in those babies.
In January, Karasik traveled to Tajikistan and
trained Dodojonova to use the portable eye tracker. If perception and action
are linked, and Tajik babies’ motor development is delayed relative to Western
babies, then the ability to link sights and sounds should also be delayed. The
researchers are analyzing their data now.
A Tajik child watches two cartoon pigs on a screen. Only one makes a “doink” sound when it bounces. Researchers use eye trackers to see when the child’s eyes hone in on the noisy animal, indicating that the child has learned to pair sights and sounds, a skill that may arise alongside motor ability. R. Dodojonova and Biblizzat Amonkul
Karasik and her colleagues also hope to start
collecting data on Tajik babies’ temperaments, which in infants is thought to
manifest as individual differences in reacting to events and regulating emotions.
Does restriction in a gahvora change how Tajik babies respond to people around
them or behave outside the gahvora? “Even if babies are out, they may not be
taking the opportunity to move,” Karasik says.
She plans to administer a standard temperament survey
that asks moms to answer questions over a weeklong period and covers issues
such as “How often does your baby play with a single toy or object for five to
10 minutes?” and “How often does your baby fall asleep within 10 minutes?”
The team suspects the gahvora teaches babies
restraint. Back when the project first started, Tamis-LeMonda recalls, the
researchers wanted to record babies’ cries as they were put into the gahvora — an
idea that was soon scrapped. The babies didn’t fuss or cry.
The idea that the gahvora builds traits like
patience and mindfulness resonates with Dodojonova, who has become one of the
cradle’s staunchest advocates. In recent years, she has taken to writing
pamphlets calling on mothers to continue cradling. The practice is under
threat, she says, from disposable diapers, which are now widely available, and
Tajik pediatricians who embrace Western notions that are at odds with cradle
use, such as tummy time and breastfeeding in the mother’s arms.
The gahvora teaches children that “they cannot do everything that they want,” Dodojonova says. What parent wouldn’t wish for that?