There’s no evidence that a single ‘gay gene’ exists

Instead, a combo of small genetic factors and environmental influences affects partner choice

gay couple

There’s no one “gay gene,” but many genetic, environmental, social and cultural factors may combine to influence sexual behavior, researchers say.

oneinchpunch/Shutterstock

Publication of the largest-ever study of the roles of genes in homosexual behavior is fanning the debate over whether being gay is due to genes or environment.

First reported at a genetics conference in 2018, the study found five genetic variants associated with having a same-sex sexual partner (SN: 10/20/18). But those variants, called SNPs, don’t predict people’s sexual behavior, researchers report in the Aug. 30 Science.

“There is no ‘gay gene’ that determines whether someone has same-sex partners,” says Andrea Ganna, a geneticist at the Broad Institute of MIT and Harvard and the University of Helsinki.

Family studies have suggested that genetics account for about 32 percent of heritability of homosexual behavior. But each SNP, or single nucleotide polymorphism, has a very small effect on whether someone has ever had a same-sex sexual partner, the new research found.

Taking into account all the SNPs measured in the study, including those that weren’t statistically significantly associated with same-sex behavior, explained only 8 to 25 percent of heritability of same-sex behavior. When considering just those five statistically significant SNPs, that number drops to much less than 1 percent.

But those variants could point to biological processes that are involved in choosing sex partners, the researchers say. For instance, one variant identified in the study has been linked to male-pattern baldness, and another to the ability to smell certain chemicals, which may affect sexual attraction.

“The study is a big step forward because of its huge size,” says J. Michael Bailey, a psychologist at Northwestern University in Evanston, Ill., who has worked on sexual orientation genetics but was not involved in the work. It included more than 470,000 people, dwarfing previous research.

“This is the first study that we can be pretty sure that they’ve identified genetic variants associated with an aspect of same-sex behavior,” Bailey says. “I’ve been a coauthor on some previous molecular genetic studies that were much more tenuous. I believe these results will replicate.”

But Bailey disagrees with some of the study’s conclusions. For instance, Ganna says that people who have exclusively same-sex or exclusively opposite-sex partners are genetically distinct from people who have partners of both sexes. That means that sexuality may not be a continuum from completely heterosexual to homosexual after all. So the Kinsey scale, which scores people’s sexual behavior along a spectrum with bisexuality in the middle, may need to be rethought, the researchers say.

But the Kinsey scale accurately predicts men’s arousal when shown erotic pictures of either men or women, Bailey says. That makes it a far better tool than the genetic score for predicting sexual preference, he says.

Qazi Rahman, a psychologist and sexual orientation researcher at King’s College London, has bigger quibbles with the study. “I should be really excited about this,” he says. But “despite being an ardent believer in the biological basis of sexuality, I’ve found this study problematic, and I’m not at all sure what was found and whether that holds up.” 

Rahman points to what he sees as inconsistencies in the data and possible bias in the people who volunteered to participate in the study. The study drew volunteers from two big genetic databases, the UK Biobank and the consumer DNA testing company 23andMe, and from three smaller studies. Participants answered questionnaires about how many sexual partners of each sex they had ever had. 23andMe customers also responded to questions about attraction, sexual identity and fantasies.

But only 5.5 percent of UK Biobank participants and about 1.5 percent of 23andMe’s customers joined the study. Such low participation rates could skew the results, or point to genetic variants that make people more likely to sign up for a study. “What you’re getting is genetic influences on self-selection into a study, not genetic influences on same-sex behavior,” Rahman says.

It’s legitimate to question where study participants come from, but there’s no way to know whether that bias is affecting the results, says coauthor Benjamin Neale, a geneticist at Massachusetts General Hospital in Boston and the Broad Institute.

The study wasn’t designed to address sexual orientation or identity, but the same variants associated with same-sex behavior were also associated in 23andMe participants with attraction, sexual identity and fantasies. The small contribution of genetics to sexual behavior is in line with genetic contributions to other behaviors, such as level of education attainment. “There’s a lot of room for nongenetic effects,” Bailey says.

The authors don’t disagree. The study underscores that elements of both biology and one’s environment may play roles in shaping sexual behavior, Neale says. Environmental influences may include an array of developmental, social and cultural factors that all could affect behavior, he says.

That’s true, says coauthor J. Fah Sathirapongsasuti, a computational biologist at 23andMe in Mountain View, Calif. But, he says, “just because something is not completely genetic or something has an environmental, or what we call nongenetic, component doesn’t mean it’s a choice.”

Tina Hesman Saey is the senior staff writer and reports on molecular biology. She has a Ph.D. in molecular genetics from Washington University in St. Louis and a master’s degree in science journalism from Boston University.

More Stories from Science News on Genetics

From the Nature Index

Paid Content